6s | On September 1st, 1953, |
9s | William Scoville used a hand crank and a cheap drill saw |
13s | to bore into a young man's skull, cutting away vital pieces of his brain |
17s | and sucking them out through a metal tube. |
20s | But this wasn't a scene from a horror film or a gruesome police report. |
25s | Dr. Scoville was one of the most renowned neurosurgeons of his time, |
29s | and the young man was Henry Molaison, the famous patient known as "H.M.", |
35s | whose case provided amazing insights into how our brains work. |
40s | As a boy, Henry had cracked his skull in an accident |
43s | and soon began having seizures, blacking out and losing control of bodily functions. |
49s | After enduring years of frequent episodes, and even dropping out of high school, |
53s | the desperate young man had turned to Dr. Scoville, |
57s | a daredevil known for risky surgeries. |
60s | Partial lobotomies had been used for decades to treat mental patients |
64s | based on the notion that mental functions were strictly localized |
67s | to corresponding brain areas. |
70s | Having successfully used them to reduce seizures in psychotics, |
74s | Scoville decided to remove H.M.'s hippocampus, |
77s | a part of the limbic system that was associated with emotion |
81s | but whose function was unknown. |
83s | At first glance, the operation had succeeded. |
86s | H.M.'s seizures virtually disappeared, with no change in personality, |
90s | and his IQ even improved. |
92s | But there was one problem: His memory was shot. |
96s | Besides losing most of his memories from the previous decade, |
99s | H.M. was unable to form new ones, forgetting what day it was, |
103s | repeating comments, and even eating multiple meals in a row. |
108s | When Scoville informed another expert, Wilder Penfield, of the results, |
112s | he sent a Ph.D student named Brenda Milner to study H.M. at his parents' home, |
117s | where he now spent his days doing odd chores, |
120s | and watching classic movies for the first time, over and over. |
124s | What she discovered through a series of tests and interviews |
127s | didn't just contribute greatly to the study of memory. |
130s | It redefined what memory even meant. |
133s | One of Milner's findings shed light on the obvious fact |
136s | that although H.M. couldn't form new memories, he still retained information |
141s | long enough from moment to moment to finish a sentence or find the bathroom. |
146s | When Milner gave him a random number, |
148s | he managed to remember it for fifteen minutes |
151s | by repeating it to himself constantly. |
153s | But only five minutes later, he forgot the test had even taken place. |
158s | Neuroscientists had though of memory as monolithic, |
161s | all of it essentially the same and stored throughout the brain. |
165s | Milner's results were not only the first clue for the now familiar distinction |
169s | between short-term and long-term memory, |
172s | but show that each uses different brain regions. |
176s | We now know that memory formation involves several steps. |
179s | After immediate sensory data is temporarily transcribed by neurons in the cortex, |
184s | it travels to the hippocampus, |
186s | where special proteins work to strengthen the cortical synaptic connections. |
192s | If the experience was strong enough, |
193s | or we recall it periodically in the first few days, |
196s | the hippocampus then transfers the memory back to the cortex for permanent storage. |
202s | H.M.'s mind could form the initial impressions, |
205s | but without a hippocampus to perform this memory consolidation, |
209s | they eroded, like messages scrawled in sand. |
213s | But this was not the only memory distinction Milner found. |
216s | In a now famous experiment, she asked H.M. to trace a third star |
221s | in the narrow space between the outlines of two concentric ones |
225s | while he could only see his paper and pencil through a mirror. |
229s | Like anyone else performing such an awkward task for the first time, |
232s | he did horribly. |
234s | But surprisingly, he improved over repeated trials, |
237s | even though he had no memory of previous attempts. |
241s | His unconscious motor centers remembered what the conscious mind had forgotten. |
246s | What Milner had discovered was that the declarative memory of names, dates and facts |
251s | is different from the procedural memory of riding a bicycle or signing your name. |
257s | And we now know that procedural memory |
259s | relies more on the basal ganglia and cerebellum, |
262s | structures that were intact in H.M.'s brain. |
266s | This distinction between "knowing that" and "knowing how" |
269s | has underpinned all memory research since. |
272s | H.M. died at the age of 82 after a mostly peaceful life in a nursing home. |
278s | Over the years, he had been examined by more than 100 neuroscientists, |
282s | making his the most studied mind in history. |
285s | Upon his death, his brain was preserved and scanned |
288s | before being cut into over 2000 individual slices |
292s | and photographed to form a digital map down to the level of individual neurons, |
297s | all in a live broadcast watched by 400,000 people. |
302s | Though H.M. spent most of his life forgetting things, |
304s | he and his contributions to our understanding of memory |
307s | will be remembered for generations to come. |